Search

Saturday, March 9, 2019

Conceptual and methodological perspectives in Psychology of Aging


Characterizing the Theoretical Landscape in Psychological Aging


By: Roger A. Dixon

Just as no history of a science is without the influence of the historian and his or her historiography, no review of scientific theory exists independently of the filtering lens through which the theorists read, interpret, and write (Hanson, 1958) or the historically evolving conceptual, social, professional, and scientific circumstances of the era (e.g., Kuhn, 1962; Pepper, 1970; Toulmin, 1972).

In the past in life-span psychological research, such observations have often led to discussion of scientific paradigms, meta-theories, and world views, as they applied to the study of individual development and aging (e.g., Baltes & Willis, 1977; Dixon & Lerner, 1999; Reese & Overton, 1970). This is not the present purpose for three related reasons.

First, the general lesson has been learned in that it is probably apparent to most contemporary readers that theories and research methods are informed by underlying (and often untestable) assumptions, models, metaphors, and perspectives (Overton & Reese, 1973).

Second, for this reason this particular line of theoretical-historical inquiry has not been particularly active or overtly influential in recent years, at least in the field of psychological aging. Third, one reason it has become both an acknowledged background condition and yet rarely cited or targeted for research is that the field may have moved to a post-paradigmatic period of interdisciplinary, integrative, and even pragmatic perspectives and research.

Perhaps in the earlier paradigmatic-centered period meta-theoretical differences were accentuated (if not magnified). If so, in a post-paradigmatic period meta-theoretical differences (if relevant) may be less likely to restrict or interrupt the pragmatic reconnaissance of (even small) plots of common ground and the probing expansion of these commonalities along shared conceptual and empirical boundaries.

Nevertheless, to recap briefly, the paradigmatic view has held that, because the underlying tendencies of different meta-approaches may be in fundamental conceptual competition, the derived theories may be incommensurable and the associated data collected to test the theories may be mutually unacceptable. This systemic and often static incommensurability may exist even when the research is addressed to common levels of analysis or evidently similar developmental phenomena (Dixon et al., 1991).

As a brief illustration, scholars studying the intriguing phenomena of late-life potential or adaptive success can address different (even non-overlapping) aspects from a variety of largely unshared conceptual and methodological perspectives.These include:

(a) Post-formal or dialectical operations;
(b) Naturally occurring differential trajectories and protection factors;
(c) Social-emotional regulation, adaptivity, and influences;
(d) Cognitive or self-reserve, plasticity, or expertise;
(e) Pragmatic cognitive-personal resilience or compensation; and
(f) Multiple forms of healthy or successful aging
(e.g., Baltes & Baltes, 1990; Ericsson & Smith, 1991; Labouvie-Vief, 1980; Pushkar et al., 1998; Schaie & Carstensen, 2006; Vaillant, 2002).

However, paradigm-level perspectives are viewed also as changing, fallible, modifiable, responsive to data, and adaptive (or not). In addition, such meta-theoretical perspectives may be inextricably interdisciplinary, theories may be more flexible and pluralistic, methods are definitely more comprehensive and powerful, and the research goals may become more pragmatic and integrative.

Specifically, research may become less characterized by how it contributes to a covering meta-theory or global theory. Instead, theoretically and clinically significant research may be evaluated in terms of how functional they are, regardless of the academic sources of ideas, levels of analyses (biological, individual–psychological, social–cultural), or the simplicity–complexity of results.

References:

Handbook of the Psychology of Aging, K. Warner Schaie, and Sherry L. Willis, 2011, Elsevier Inc.

Read Also:




No comments:

Post a Comment